運用心率變化評估焦慮:以幼兒說謊為例,ERICDATA高等教育知識庫
高等教育出版
熱門: 朱丽彬  黃光男  王美玲  王善边  曾瓊瑤  崔雪娟  
高等教育出版
首頁 臺灣期刊   學校系所   學協會   民間出版   大陸/海外期刊   政府機關   學校系所   學協會   民間出版   DOI註冊服務
閱讀全文
篇名
運用心率變化評估焦慮:以幼兒說謊為例
並列篇名
Recognizing Young Children’s Anxiety While Lying: Utilizing Heart Rate Data From a Smart Bracelet
作者 邱淑惠莊胡祖德
中文摘要
本研究旨在運用心率變化偵測學齡前幼兒的焦慮變化,採實驗情境的研究設計,於取得家長同意後,邀請91名5.43-6.49歲幼兒參與有機會說謊的實驗情境,並以運動心率手環記錄幼兒的心率變化。本研究比較幼兒在實驗各期的心率變化,主要目的為:一、探究說謊與誠實組幼兒在實驗過程包含基期、遊戲期、獨處期、回答期共四期的心率變化;二、利用心率手環提供的數據設定警戒值,以區別平靜時(基期)與焦慮時(獨處期、回答期)的狀態。研究發現:一、說謊與誠實組幼兒的心率表現無顯著差異,但幼兒在實驗各期有組內差異。幼兒在獨處期與回答期的心率顯著高於基期,支持以心率變化偵測幼兒焦慮變化的可行性;此外,要區別幼兒是否處於焦慮狀態,除考量該名幼兒當下的心率值外,亦應同時參考幼兒在基期心率的平均值與變化幅度。二、在實務應用上,以心率手環提供的有限數據(某段期間的心率最高值與平均值),可取代心率手環沒有提供但學術上常使用的標準差,為幼兒設定一個方便估算的警戒值,提高實務應用的便利性。
英文摘要

Motivation and Purpose

Preschool children and those with special needs often struggle to articulate their anxiety clearly. Identifying anxiety through a simple, non-invasive, and valid physiological indicator would be invaluable in educational, counseling, and academic research fields. This study aims to propose practical applications such as using a smart bracelet with alert settings for children with emotional regulation difficulties. The monitors could warn teachers or the children themselves when heart rates exceed a specific threshold, indicating potential emotional distress. This study compares the heart rate variations of preschool children across different phases of an experiment. The primary objectives are: (1) to investigate the heart rate changes in lying versus honest children during the four experimental phases (baseline, play, alone, response), and (2) to utilize data from heart rate monitors to establish threshold values, distinguishing between calm (baseline) and anxious (alone, response) states.

Literature Review

The Polyvagal Theory describes how the autonomic nervous system responds to various environmental contexts, influencing subsequent physiological responses such as heart rate (Porges, 2009). When environmental threats increase, the ventral vagal complex is suppressed, leading to the dominance of the sympathetic nervous system, resulting in an accelerated heart rate. Children as young as three and a half years old begin to lie (Evans & Lee, 2011). In experimental settings, spontaneous lying behaviors in children are often studied through a temptation scenario, such as leaving the child alone after instructing them not to peek at a noise-making toy (Lewis et al., 1989) or to refrain from looking at a covered card to win a game (Zhao et al., 2017). Lying can induce anxiety and heart rate changes, with heart rate variability serving as an indicator of anxiety levels.

Methodology

This study invited 91 children (45 girls and 46 boys) aged 5.43 to 6.49 years (M = 5.93) to participate in a game. Based on the card game scenario from Zhao et al. (2017), the experimental setup was divided into four phases: baseline (sitting quietly before the game), play (introduction of the game), alone (researcher leaves the room), and response (researcher asks if the child peeked at the cards). Children were categorized into lying and honest groups based on their spontaneous behavior.

Research Results

1. Overall Heart Rate Variability: The heart rate variability was significantly higher during the alone and response phases compared to the play and baseline phases. The findings support the feasibility of using heart rate to detect anxiety in young children.

2. Distinguishing Lying and Honest Groups: The heart rate variations in different phases could not reliably distinguish between lying and honest children. This could be due to greater within-subject variability in heart rates than between-group differences or some honest children experiencing anxiety in the later phases.

3. Individual Heart Rate Comparison: Comparing a child’s heart rate against their baseline average and standard deviation is essential to determine anxiety states. This supports the notion that heart rate and its variability are influenced by individual physiological traits (Meijer & Verschuere, 2018). The variability in baseline heart rates predicted subsequent heart rate changes, with greater baseline variability indicating a higher likelihood of anxiety-induced heart rate increases.

4. Effect Sizes of Phase and Baseline Heart Rate: Both the phase factor and the baseline heart rate standard deviation (as a covariate) had small effect sizes on subsequent heart rates. The effect size of the phase (representing context) was slightly higher than that of the covariate (representing physiological traits). This suggests that situational factors have a more substantial impact on heart rate changes than personal traits in this study.

5. Maximum Heart Rate Variability: The highest heart rate variability during the alone phase distinguished between the honest and lying groups. Honest children showed greater variability, possibly due to internal struggles and anxiety while deciding whether to peek, or because some waited calmly without intent to cheat. The lying group was more uniformly anxious about deciding to cheat.

6. Minimum Heart Rate: Minimum heart rates showed significant differences across all phases, indicating a consistent upward trend. According to The Polyvagal Theory (Porges, 1995), as anxiety increases, the minimum heart rate stabilizes and rises unless controlled by the dorsal vagal complex, which lowers the heart rate. Therefore, examining maximum and minimum heart rates, in addition to the average, provides richer information.

7. Adjusting Heart Rate Alert Thresholds: Results indicated that using the formula “baseline maximum heart rate + 0.2 * (baseline maximum heart rate - baseline average)” produced similar effects to Lee et al.’s (2010) “baseline average + 2SD” but both were slightly lenient. In practical applications, thresholds can be flexibly adjusted according to individual children’s traits or specific usage purposes, tailoring appropriate alert values for each child.

Discussion and Recommendations

This study employed an experimental setting to tempt children to lie. The findings show that children’s heart rates significantly increased when they had the opportunity to cheat and subsequently lie to cover up their actions, compared to their calm states. This demonstrates the practical utility of using heart rate changes to detect emotional shifts in children. Additionally, the study found that real-time data from heart rate bracelets, including the average and maximum heart rates during calm states, can be used to set alert thresholds with similar effectiveness to the standards used by Lee et al. (2010).

Exploring the use of heart rate to detect anxiety can expand its application range. For example, during play therapy with traumatized children, monitoring their emotional fluctuations with heart rate bracelets can be beneficial. Similarly, pairing heart rate bracelets with behavioral training techniques for children with emotional disorders can help manage anxiety. Setting alert thresholds on the bracelets can teach children to pause activities and use pre-taught emotional regulation strategies when alerted. It can also prompt caregivers to identify environmental factors that may cause anxiety, enabling preventive measures.

Most children’s heart rates did not significantly spike, indicating that the experimental setting induced only mild anxiety. However, in practical applications, severe anxiety might cause the dorsal vagal complex to take over the autonomic nervous system, leading to a sudden drop in heart rate. Therefore, setting minimum heart rate thresholds is also crucial. Lee et al. (2010) used the baseline average heart rate minus two standard deviations as an alert threshold. While this study’s data do not provide specific recommendations for this, practitioners can experiment with this approach. Additionally, the experience of lying might predict heart rate changes across different phases, a variable not considered in this study due to a lack of relevant data. Future research should include this variable. Lastly, factors other than anxiety, such as excitement, can also increase heart rates, which may explain the significant rise in heart rates during the play phase in this study.

起訖頁 115-142
關鍵詞 心率心率手環焦慮說謊學齡前幼兒heart ratesmart braceletanxietylyingpreschoolers
刊名 教育科學研究期刊  
期數 202503 (70:1期)
出版單位 國立臺灣師範大學
DOI 10.6209/JORIES.202503_70(1).0004   複製DOI
QR Code
該期刊
上一篇
To Be or Not to Be? 教職召喚與專業認同對藝術大學師資生生涯決策自我效能、結果期待及生涯希望感的調節效果分析
該期刊
下一篇
高敏感特質中高齡學習者面對數位學習壓力及疫情焦慮之學習滿意度與後疫情調適:復原力之中介效果

高等教育知識庫  新書優惠  教育研究月刊  全球重要資料庫收錄  

教師服務
合作出版
期刊徵稿
聯絡高教
高教FB
讀者服務
圖書目錄
教育期刊
訂購服務
活動訊息
數位服務
高等教育知識庫
國際資料庫收錄
投審稿系統
DOI註冊
線上購買
高點網路書店 
元照網路書店
博客來網路書店
教育資源
教育網站
國際教育網站
關於高教
高教簡介
出版授權
合作單位
知識達 知識達 知識達 知識達 知識達 知識達
版權所有‧轉載必究 Copyright2011 高等教育文化事業股份有限公司  All Rights Reserved
服務信箱:edubook@edubook.com.tw 台北市館前路 26 號 6 樓 Tel:+886-2-23885899 Fax:+886-2-23892500